banner



What Type Of Animals Live In The Sahel?

Introduction

Deserts and semi-deserts are characteristically open environments with limited vegetation coverage. They are harsh environments, severely limited by almanac precipitation (Holt et al. 2013, Vale & Brito 2015), and therefore, desert organisms have been studied from the perspective of adaptation to arid weather (Kotler & Dark-brown 1988, Brito et al. 2014). Due to a lack of vegetation (or surface structures), exposed desert habitats provide an opportunity to understand natural option under extreme conditions (Die 1947, Brown et al. 1988, Kotler & Brown 1988, Nachman et al. 2003, Hoekstra et al. 2006, Brito et al. 2014, Boratyński et al. 2014, Bleicher et al. 2018).

Selection, broadly defined as differential fitness of individuals caused past differences expressed in their phenotypes, is a primal mechanism of evolution driving adaptive modify (Bong 2008). In this process, natural sorting of the genetic constituents of individuals at the phenotypic level occurs (Reznick 2016). Selection tin can shape phenotypic variation due to covariation, for example, between prey phenotypic features and predation pressure level, as in desert rodents (Kotler & Brown 1988, Hoekstra et al. 2006, Boratyński et al. 2017, Bleicher et al. 2018). Therefore, predator-induced pick tin drive evolution of casualty anti-predatory adaptations, such as high mobility and saltation behaviour (Alhajeri 2016), bullar hypertrophy (i.e. an auditory morphology in bone structure) aiding auditory sensation and detection of approaching predators (Alhajeri & Steppan 2018), and phenotype-environment convergence for visual crypsis (Caro 2005, Boratyński et al. 2014).

In this review, we focus on North African desert rodents and summarize the current understanding of visual camouflage adaptation in the Sahara-Sahel rodent community. Spanning a large biogeographic extent in North Africa (Fig. 1), and comprising a marked taxonomic diversity (Alhajeri et al. 2015, Boratyński et al. 2017), the Sahara-Sahelian rodent customs represents one of the most compelling cases of cover-up adaptation in the wild (Nokelainen et al. 2020). As the community faces extreme (in terms of temperature and dryness) and heterogeneous selection pressures (Kotler & Dark-brown 1988), testing of ecological predictions of ongoing climate and landscape changes is possible. We hash out how this report system tin can contribute to our understanding of the processes that shape accommodation under ongoing environmental change.

Fig. 1.

Rodent species geographic locations (blackness dots) obtained from the Global Biodiversity Information Facility indicating express data for most of the Sahara-Sahel (left panel). Inserted outline of Commonwealth of australia (scarlet) is presented on the same calibration equally the Sahara-Sahel (yellow), for size comparing. Average annual temperature range (superlative-right) and average yearly precipitation in Due north Africa (bottom-right).

img-z2-7_01.jpg

Land-of-the-art

One of the nigh pervasive selection pressures in open habitats is selection driving evolution of anti-predator adaptations, particularly pick for visual concealment from predators or cover-up (Stevens & Merilaita 2009, Boratyński et al. 2014, 2017, Merilaita et al. 2017). In social club to avert detection and recognition, animals ofttimes share visual characteristics, including lightness, colour and pattern, with that of their environs (Cott 1940, Stevens & Merilaita 2011, Nokelainen & Stevens 2016). Camouflage is a widespread anti-predator strategy found across many taxa and is non restricted to desert environments. Yet, as camouflage helps animals to escape from predators, it is especially of import to survival in exposed habitats, where opportunities for hiding in physical structures (such as trees or vegetation) are express (Die 1947, Kaufman 1974, Vignieri et al. 2010). It is worth mentioning that to counteract the natural lack of shelters, many rodents can make burrows, which helps them to escape predators (Rios & Álvarez-Castañeda 2012), or are nocturnal.

Camouflage in the Sahara-Sahel desert rodent customs presents a fine-tuned example of background matching in geographically widespread taxa (Boratyński et al. 2014, 2017). Both the colour and the pattern of animals are correlated with their respective backgrounds and vision modelling has shown that their camouflage is effective confronting both mammalian and avian vision models (Nokelainen et al. 2020). Animal-to-background differences are more often than not low in Sahara-Sahel rodents at big spatial scales (i.e. such as when animals are being compared between all the backgrounds that the species uses), supporting the hypothesis of a generalist camouflage strategy. However, some species (e.thou. Jaculus hirtipes) may friction match all-time their local habitat (Nokelainen et al. 2020), suggesting the importance of behaviour in improving camouflage and habitat specialization (Stevens & Ruxton 2019). Still, a controlled verification of that process is required. It has also been shown that cover-up has evolved repeatedly in Sahara-Sahel rodents, fifty-fifty amid related Gerbillus species (Boratyński et al. 2017), which suggests that it is an evolutionary labile accommodation, and perchance linked to the repeated changes between semi-arid to hyper-arid habitats over the history of the Sahara-Sahel (Brito et al. 2014, Boratyński et al. 2017, Alhajeri & Steppan 2018).

The ongoing environmental changes in the Sahara-Sahel betrayal organisms to spatially and temporally heterogeneous choice (Kotler & Chocolate-brown 1988, Brito et al. 2014). Thus, many species face the constant risk of mismatching their habitat (Merilaita et al. 1999, 2001, Michalis et al. 2017). In theory, animals may evolve several optimal camouflage patterns (Merilaita et al. 1999, 2001, Michalis et al. 2017), potentially specializing to match their well-nigh oftentimes used micro-habitat. Notwithstanding, if several habitat types are sufficiently similar, an result would be a reduction in the conflict between matching different background types, and thus, a generalist strategy may outperform specialism (Merilaita et al. 2001, Houston et al. 2007, Hughes et al. 2019). In this context, merchandise-offs between specialist and generalist camouflage adaptation can emerge from species' differences in exploratory and personality traits, which will influence the range of micro-habitats visited. While there are potential benefits of exploratory and assuming behaviour (eastward.one thousand. higher chances of encountering novel resource), the predation costs may also exist loftier (Dingemanse & Reale 2005, Nicolaus et al. 2016, but meet Moiron et al. 2020).

Co-evolution between complex behavioural strategies likewise every bit spatial and temporal scales of adaptations are theoretically predicted (Stevens & Ruxton 2019), but less often tested in wild fauna communities. Every bit Sahara-Sahel desert rodents compromise cover-up accuracy across different habitat types (Nokelainen et al. 2020), they present a good model to investigate how phenotype-environment matching evolves in the community at variable spatial as well as temporal scales (Boratyński et al. 2014, 2017). For example, some rodents may specialize on specific micro-habitats and integration of colour quantification methods may testify useful in revealing habitat specialization and intracommunity interactions. Recent methodological advances enable the measurement of animal-to-groundwork matching more objectively (Troscianko & Stevens 2015, Van Den Berg et al. 2019) and the quantification of how camouflage may deceive ecologically relevant sensory systems.

Perspectives

Nosotros outline how desert rodents are a particularly valuable study system to tackle the post-obit open questions, all of which are applicable to desert habitat: 1) How do animals cope with habitat heterogeneity over unlike spatial scales? two) How does seasonality determine the evolution of cover-up? 3) How does rodent behaviour (e.g. through mobility, dominance structure, life-history strategies) dictate the efficacy of anti-predator strategies? iv) How does behavioural (east.g. diurnal, crepuscular or nocturnal) and cerebral processing of predators facilitate cover-up efficacy?Although these questions may apply to other environments and/or study systems, nosotros focus on desert rodents of Sahara-Sahel as a working instance.

How do animals cope with habitat heterogeneity over different spatial scales?

The Sahara-Sahel region is a vast biogeographic entity in Due north Africa (Fig. one). Species occupying this environment are assumed to accept variable home ranges and mobility, and thus confront different camouflage requirements. The terrain does not consist of pure sand habitats; rather, in addition to dunes, at that place are seasonal river habitats of varied levels of mud, clay and rock, shrublands, rocky plates and outcrops, combined with altitudinal variation in habitat type (Brito et al. 2014, Campos & Brito 2018). Consequently, desert rodents have repeatedly evolved variable levels of background matching (Boratyński et al. 2017). Animals may accept adopted versatile camouflage tactics and behaviours where effective darkening requires matching unlike types of lightness, colour and pattern (i.e. substrate granularity). For example, they may have evolved a generalist strategy, which can exist viewed as "imperfect camouflage" (Hughes et al. 2019), representing a compromise to match unlike visual backgrounds (Nokelainen et al. 2020). Alternatively, they may become cover-up specialists, which should constrain the location and/or habitat utilize of a given species through increased vulnerability (Merilaita et al. 1999, 2001, Kjernsmo & Merilaita 2012, Michalis et al. 2017). Camouflage specialists are predicted to be less vulnerable than cover-up generalists within the habitat to which they have specialized, simply the specialist species are constrained in their use of the habitat types, as they suffer greater vulnerability to predators outside their specialized habitat.

How does seasonality decide the evolution of camouflage in arid environments?

Although seasonality has a well-documented touch on animal cover-up (e.g. through seasonal polyphenism), how seasonality in desert landscapes may influence camouflage efficacy is less well understood. The other issue on drylands is that low predictability (or repeatability) of seasons, eastward.yard. extended periods without rainfall, sometimes lasting several years, is not unusual (Foley et al. 2003, Dardel et al. 2013). While a item habitat may superficially appear similar based just on visual appearance, the resources available may change through the seasons (i.e. moisture vs. dry season) and this alter may drive animals to move, thereby exposing them to different camouflage requirements. Thus, we may again predict that species forced to utilise large areas should be more generalist habitat users, whereas species that are more than sedentary may have evolved to utilize detail types of habitat (e.k. mountain endemics or tight niche utilizers).

How does rodent behaviour dictate the efficacy of anti-predator strategies?

Although cover-up has traditionally been considered as a phenotypic trait to be associated with the local surround, evidence is accumulating that behaviour in a particular habitat may play a crucial part how well camouflage works (Stevens & Ruxton 2019). For instance, motion has been shown to exist an upshot for camouflage considering predators tend to focus their attention on moving targets (Hall et al. 2013). When detected, a mobile species can either endeavour to evade predators past running to a shelter (Fig. 2a), or by manoeuvring while escaping (Fig. 2b). Camouflage in species adopting this behaviour should be poorer than in those species that evade predators by remaining stationary and relying on "freeze" behaviour (Fig. 2c). In addition, when the movement of prey ceases it is important to terminate in a location that minimises vulnerability. Besides, more bold (or dominant and exploratory) animals may be exposed more than shy (or subordinate) animals but, on the other hand, more dominant individuals may exist meliorate competitors and may force subordinates to motion. It is possible that behavioural strategies mirror complex life-history interactions that may be difficult to tease apart without integration of phylogenetic, experimental and statistical approaches (Moiron et al. 2020).

Fig. 2.

Visual representation of animals and their respective micro-habitats. Panels (a-g) illustrate dissimilar visual appearances of animals and how well fur colouration matches their corresponding background colour and design. Note the microhabitat granularity. For objective photography purposes reflectance standards were included. a) Gerbillus nigeriae, 21 km NW from Kenkossa, Mauritania, 20.eleven.2012; b) G. amoenus, 85 km E from Msied, Kingdom of morocco, 07.02.2016; c) G. amoenus, 31 km NW from Ouadane, Mauritania, 31.10.2011; d) Pachyuromys duprasi, 17 km SW from Aouint Lahna, Morocco, 03.02.2016; e) Jaculus jaculus, iv km S from El Hagounia, Kingdom of morocco, 11.02.2016; f) J. hirtipes, 35 km E from Abteh, Kingdom of morocco, 09.02.2016; k) G. gerbillus, 120 km Due west from Choum, Islamic republic of mauritania, 24.10.2011 (photo Z. Boratyński).

img-z4-8_01.jpg

To sum up, nosotros predict that more mobile animals, and those with large abode ranges spanning heterogenous environments, should adopt a more generalist and/or compromise camouflage strategy. Less mobile, more than sedentary or less competitive species should rely on more specialized camouflage (Merilaita et al. 2017, Fennell et al. 2019, Hughes et al. 2019). Animals having larger home ranges would more than likely have a need to cross several different types of micro-habitats in comparison to less mobile individuals/species with smaller ranges. It would be valuable to test how different proxies of mobility (such as power to saltation, length of the hind foot and/or tail), as used in previous studies (Alhajeri et al. 2015, Alhajeri 2016, Alhajeri & Steppan 2018), correlate with the degree of crypsis and/or with different personality types (Moiron et al. 2020).

Fig. 3.

Representation of 3 behavioural strategies that may influence how camouflage counters predator perception in combination with motion: a) run and hibernate (Gerbillus sp., 43.5 km West from Kenkossa, Mauritania, 01.09.2015), b) run fast and manoeuvre sharply during evasion (Jaculus hirtipes, 185 km SE from Dakhla, Morocco, 06.01.2018), or c) sit down and wait (Pachyuromys duprasi, 49 km S from Assa, Morocco, 07.02.2016). Each strategy may accept consequences for how accurately the rodent matches its background and, as a event, the effectiveness of its camouflage in the respective environments (photo Z. Boratyński).

img-z5-2_01.jpg

How do behaviour and cognitive processing of predators facilitate camouflage efficacy?

Animals utilise their vision to acquire data or to reduce uncertainty in their environs (Maynard Smith & Harper 2004, Stevens 2013). Thus, reducing the capability of a predator to acquire this information reliably is crucial for prey using visual camouflage (Mokkonen & Lindstedt 2015, Merilaita et al. 2017). The visual advent of the prey in a given environment is of import, but as well how predators acquire and process visual information (i.e. retinal sensitivities to different wavelengths, visual vigil, neural transmission and processing of information in the visual cortex of the encephalon) will influence their decision making in prey search (Endler & Mappes 2017, Cuthill et al. 2019). The process of "seeing" color tin exist simplified into the following stages: viewing weather condition, color representation, perception and cognitive processing of the receiver (White & Kemp 2015). Changes in whatever of these can change the efficacy of camouflage, favour unlike camouflage tactics and shape the strength of selection (Endler 1992, Cost 2017).

Fig. 4.

Schematic representation of the differences among remote sensing devices used in habitat colouration studies, in terms of spatial, temporal and spectral resolutions. Differences are exemplified with satellite images obtained from distinct sources a) MODIS-Terra, image acquired in 2019/05/27; b) Landsat eight, paradigm acquired in 2019/05/22; c) Sentinel-two, image acquired in 2019/05/27) and with an aerial photograph d) obtained by an UAV (image acquired in 2020/01/06). Satellite images were obtained through EarthExplorer interface from the U.s. Geological Survey ( https://earthexplorer.usgs.gov/).

img-z5-8_01.jpg

In deserts, the nigh notable changes in viewing conditions are mostly those over the course of the mean solar day; diurnal predators have much more light bachelor for visual processing in comparison to nocturnal animals, which take to cope with (scotopic) depression light intensities. Broadly speaking, matching the colour spectrum is assumed more of import for diurnal and crepuscular animals, whereas luminance matching may exist more important for nocturnal animals (Kelber 2006, Kelber & Lind 2010). Information technology should be noted that although nosotros can brand detailed measurements of colour and reflectance to build up receiver-independent estimates of animate being-to-background matching, the visual perception of predators may differ; visual vigil varies profoundly among species; as does their ability to process light at unlike wavelengths (Kelber et al. 2003, Caves et al. 2018). Humans are trichromats (i.e. we come across three unlike chromatic ranges perceived every bit red, green and blue) and accept proficient visual acuity. Many mammals are dichromats, lacking long wavelength receptors and abrupt visual acuity, which tin can improve the efficacy of certain cover-up types against mammalian predators. Birds are typically tetrachromats, can procedure the ultraviolet part of the spectrum, and the visual acuity of raptors outperforms ours, driving strong selection for crypsis on day-active prey. However, most desert rodents are crepuscular or nocturnal. For these species, owls pose a detail chance (San-jose et al. 2019), as they can procedure luminance information in depression light conditions (Wu et al. 2016). Finally, visual input lone cannot explain why certain cover-up types predominate. As visual information is processed through the psycho-physiological landscape of the receiver (Stevens 2007, Skelhorn & Rowe 2016), it is plausible that higher-level cognitive processing and predator psychology influences the effectiveness of camouflage.

Prospects

Electric current methodologies permit detailed insight into wild rodents: i) phenotypic variation in fur colouration, two) visual structure of their habitats, 3) individual variation in shyness/disrespect and exploratory behaviours, iv) mobility and home range sizes, and v) targeting inheritance and expression mechanisms of the in a higher place traits, thus allowing a reconstruction of their evolutionary history and testing their adaptive significance. These can be applied to natural populations on individuals in their native habitats.

Beast colouration tin be accurately estimated in a controlled way, even under field conditions (Fig. 3). To measure out the level of camouflage adaptation, digital images of an animal's dorsum, along with the background where they were captured and a colour standard (e.g. Spectralon, X-Rite), can exist taken with a full spectrum camera (Stevens et al. 2007, Johnsen 2016). Saved images in RAW format, corrected for white balance, can exist analysed with open up-source software (e.g. Paradigm Scale and Analysis Toolbox, Image-J;  www.jolyon.co.britain; www.empiricalimaging.com; Troscianko & Stevens 2015, Van Den Berg et al. 2019) based on quantified photographic camera responses to a set of grayness standards (Stevens et al. 2007, Troscianko et al. 2017, Price et al. 2019). Multispectral image analysis is a powerful tool to study animal colouration. Information technology allows the analysis of entire visual scenes, such equally fur colouration compared to background (Fig. 3), and utilizes several colour metrics to enable quantification of colours and patterns simultaneously (Gómez et al. 2018, Hawkes et al. 2019).

Habitat appearance in variable spatial and temporal scales tin exist estimated with bachelor remote sensing methodologies based on aerial and satellite imagery (Fig. 4). To mensurate the spatial scale of cover-up adaptation, e.g. habitat specialist versus habitat generalist strategies, aerial images of a mosaic of habitats along with a color standard (e.m. big X-Rite), can be taken using commercially available, and affordable, drones (e.g. Mavic Pro, equipped with DJI camera; Fig. 4). The series of digital images permit analysis of the unabridged visual scene, composed of a mosaic of habitats, in a similar fashion to analyses of standard digital images taken with hand held cameras. Camouflage over a big geographical calibration tin can be studied using publicly available satellite imagery (e.g. NASA Landsat satellite series or MODIS Terra, both available at EarthExplorer interface from the United States Geological Survey:  https://earthexplorer.usgs.gov/; Fig. 4). Low cloud coverage images should be selected and appropriate atmospheric corrections should exist applied to standardize satellite imagery (Chander et al. 2009). The time periods of satellite images, and their spatial resolution (e.one thousand. 30 thou for Landsat) tin can be selected to target specific inquiry questions, e.grand. related to seasonal variation or long-term habitat changes observed over years/ decades, or specific spatial relevance for given species (Boratyński et al. 2014, 2017).

Fig. 5.

Conceptual representation of an open loonshit test to study fauna behaviour, and its visualization: a) an automatically recorded track for a bold animal, with mobility in both the centre and edges of the arena; and b) a rails for a shy fauna, with mobility mainly at the edges of the arena; c) results for selected variables after tracking the bold animal (from panel a); and d) variables after tracking the shy beast (from panel b).

img-z7-2_01.jpg

Behavioural patterns and creature personality types tin can be studied in wild individuals in combination with in situ cover-up experiments (Fig. 5). To approximate how personality and exploratory behaviours can interfere with camouflage accuracy measured over variable spatial scales (i.e. if personality correlates with background option, or if species have evolved habitat specialization), a standard rodent open field test can be applied (Cummins & Walsh 1976, Gould et al. 2009, Montiglio et al. 2010, Šíchová et al. 2014, Mazzamuto et al. 2019). Trials can even be performed inside a modified field vehicle, if necessary, and affordable cameras can be used to record videos of animal behaviour (east.g. GoPro). Available software facilitates quantification of various aspects of beast behaviour, such as mobility and shyness-disrespect, using commercial automatic tracking systems (e.g. Ethovision XT, Noldus) or their open-source alternatives (e.1000. EthoWatcher or OpenControl; Aguiar et al. 2007). Individual traits related to running speed, distances and trajectories of movements can exist related to spatial extent and accurateness of camouflage. Accurate GPS tracking, with microtransmitters, will allow non only the analysis of point data (e.g. capturing locations) to examination the lucifer betwixt phenotype and habitat, but also provide a measure of habitat selection in free-ranging animals through behavioural analyses. The ICARUS system is currently being tested, and when available (and affordable) it will revolutionize behavioural, spatial and mobility studies in wild populations, including small animals ( http://www.icarusinitiative.org; Wikelski et al. 2007, Pennisi 2011, Wikelski 2013, Wikelski & Tertitski 2016).

Associated with ecological research, molecular studies tin can provide a much broader perspective if conducted in weather that are as natural as possible (Mitchell-Olds et al. 2008, Pardo-Diaz et al. 2015). Transcriptomic and genomic tools developed for model laboratory rodents can potentially be practical to wild Sahara-Sahelian species. In this fashion, gene expression variation in fur color and behavioural traits between habitat specialists and generalists can be investigated. Sensitive samples tin can be preserved in the field in specialized solutions (east.k. RNAlater) and mobile freezers. Rodent genomes (model and not-model) are readily bachelor to aid the mapping of novel sequences of wild species. Subtle differences in cistron expression patterns between related species can exist now tackled with co-expression network analyses (Pardo-Diaz et al. 2015, Voigt et al. 2017, Gysi et al. 2018, Fu et al. 2019). Evolutionary-ecological functional genomics let identification of the molecular mechanisms underlining camouflage, and enables targeting coevolution of "non-visible" traits (eastward.g. physiology or immunology correlates). Genomics information can assistance phylogenetic reconstruction, especially in diverse and problematic groups, such equally Gerbillus rodents, where traditional marker based phylogenetics has proven difficult (Ndiaye et al. 2016a, b). Accurately reconstructed species relatedness is imperative for comparative analyses of coevolution betwixt species' quantitative traits (Weber & Agrawal 2012, Boratyński 2020), such as mobility, cover-up and habitat use.

Discussion

Nosotros are entering an heady time when integrative studies can be applied to diverse animal communities and wild populations. African rodent communities inhabiting open habitats, such as the Sahara-Sahel or Namibian desert, constitute excellent model systems. The value of Sahara-Sahel rodents is that the written report system presents an opportunity to examination adaptations facilitating species' persistence in a mosaic of habitats over dynamically irresolute weather condition (i.e. environmental change). Human-induced climate change has had negative effects on biodiversity worldwide (Bellard et al. 2012). Whenever the velocity of change is too fast for development to keep footstep, individuals risk becoming poorly adapted to their environment, which might lead to population declines and extinction (Urban 2015). The velocity of climatic change is highest in barren areas (Loarie et al. 2009), merely behavioural flexibility may allow organisms to respond apace to environmental change, and thereby facilitate survival under changed weather condition (Catullo et al. 2019). While arid areas, such equally the Sahara-Sahel, are characterized by low rainfall that limits biodiversity (Huxman et al. 2004), these environments can be surprisingly various in habitats and species, and back up loftier levels of endemism (Brito et al. 2016, Guerreiro et al. 2016).

The remoteness of many arid areas has hitherto constrained research. Limited research on remote deserts ways that there is a hazard that arid-adapted biodiversity might disappear earlier being described (Brito et al. 2014, Vale & Brito 2015). However, field expeditions are at present existence undertaken to collect information on individual and species characteristics, including behaviour, to considerately record the visual appearance (e.one thousand. lightness, colour, design granularity) of animals and their habitat. This approach is supported by technological advances, such as access to free and high-resolution satellite imagery, the availability of small, rugged and high-quality cameras for recording animal behaviour nether field conditions, and video tracking systems for automatic monitoring of behaviour. As quantifying camouflage has become straightforward and accessible, information technology is now a matter of applying the available tools to wildlife enquiry in social club to understand how biologically relevant receivers may perceive animals and shape phenotypic evolution. Advances in any of these areas of inquiry volition guide our understanding of the environmental of adaptation.

Camouflage is a blended phenotypic trait that is adamant past the genetic composition of its carrier and mirroring a complex variation in morphology (e.grand. allometry, structures and/or colours), behaviour (e.g. mobility level, boldness/shyness) and physiology (e.thou. capability for mobility or thermoregulation), all of which can be targeted by selection in a given environment. To understand what maintains heritable variation in the wild we demand integrated inquiry on phenotypic evolution (Caro & Mallarino 2002), including camouflage, which is ubiquitous in desert rodent communities. Studies of widely distributed animal communities, such equally those spanning the Sahara-Sahel, will permit broad conclusions to be drawn on the processes driving adaptation and providing a better understanding well-nigh how natural variety evolves.

Source: https://bioone.org/journals/journal-of-vertebrate-biology/volume-69/issue-2/jvb.20007/Camouflage-in-arid-environments--the-case-of-Sahara-Sahel/10.25225/jvb.20007.full

Posted by: motleymainst.blogspot.com

0 Response to "What Type Of Animals Live In The Sahel?"

Post a Comment

Iklan Atas Artikel

Iklan Tengah Artikel 1

Iklan Tengah Artikel 2

Iklan Bawah Artikel